The original article by Malcolm Gladwell in the New Yorker:
Forty years ago, in a paper in American Scientist, Herbert Simon and William Chase drew one of the most famous conclusions in the study of expertise:
There are no instant experts in chess—certainly no instant masters or grandmasters. There appears not to be on record any case (including Bobby Fischer) where a person reached grandmaster level with less than about a decade's intense preoccupation with the game. We would estimate, very roughly, that a master has spent perhaps 10,000 to 50,000 hours staring at chess positions…
In the years that followed, an entire field within psychology grew up devoted to elaborating on Simon and Chase’s observation—and researchers, time and again, reached the same conclusion: it takes a lot of practice to be good at complex tasks. After Simon and Chase’s paper, for example, the psychologist John Hayes looked at seventy-six famous classical composers and found that, in almost every case, those composers did not create their greatest work until they had been composing for at least ten years. (The sole exceptions: Shostakovich and Paganini, who took nine years, and Erik Satie, who took eight.)
This is the scholarly tradition I was referring to in my book “Outliers,” when I wrote about the “ten-thousand-hour rule.” No one succeeds at a high level without innate talent, I wrote: “achievement is talent plus preparation.” But the ten-thousand-hour research reminds us that "the closer psychologists look at the careers of the gifted, the smaller the role innate talent seems to play and the bigger the role preparation seems to play." In cognitively demanding fields, there are no naturals. Nobody walks into an operating room, straight out of a surgical rotation, and does world-class neurosurgery. And second—and more crucially for the theme of Outliers—the amount of practice necessary for exceptional performance is so extensive that people who end up on top need help. They invariably have access to lucky breaks or privileges or conditions that make all those years of practice possible. As examples, I focussed on the countless hours the Beatles spent playing strip clubs in Hamburg and the privileged, early access Bill Gates and Bill Joy got to computers in the nineteen-seventies. “He has talent by the truckload,” I wrote of Joy. “But that’s not the only consideration. It never is.”
Recently, there has been some confusion about this argument. Some of the critiques are just bewildering. Here, for example, is a passage from an article in Time a few months ago, which makes me think that there is another Malcolm Gladwell out there, with far more eccentric views than mine, who put on a Halloween wig and somehow conned his way into the Time Life Building:
Based on research suggesting that practice is the essence of genius, best-selling author Malcolm Gladwell popularized the idea that 10,000 hours of appropriately guided practice was “the magic number of greatness,” regardless of a person’s natural aptitude. With enough practice, he claimed in his book Outliers, anyone could achieve a level of proficiency that would rival that of a professional. It was just a matter of putting in the time.
Regardless of a person’s natural aptitude?
A more thoughtful response comes from David Epstein in his fascinating new book The Sports Gene. Epstein’s key point is that the ten-thousand-hour idea must be understood as an average. For example, both he and I discuss the same study by the psychologist K. Anders Ericsson that looked at students studying violin at the elite Music Academy of West Berlin. I was interested in the general finding, which was that the best violinists, on average and over time, practiced muchmore than the good ones. In other words, within a group of talented people, what separated the best from the rest was how long and how intently they worked. Epstein points out, however, that there is a fair amount of variation behind that number—suggesting that some violinists may use their practice time so efficiently that they reach a high degree of excellence more quickly. It’s an important point. There are seventy-three great composers who took at least ten years to flourish. But there is much to be learned as well from Shostakovich, Paganini, and Satie.
Epstein makes two other arguments that are worth mentioning. The first is about chess. He cites a study by Guillermo Campitelli and Fernand Gobet of a hundred and four competitive chess players. Epstein says that they found that the average time it took to reach “master” status was eleven thousand hours—but that one player reached that level in just three thousand hours. This is variation on an extreme scale. Does that mean that in chess “naturals” really do exist? I’m not so sure. Epstein is talking about chess masters—the lowest of the four categories of recognized chess experts. (It’s Division II chess.) Grandmasters—the highest level—are a different story. Robert Howard, of the University of New South Wales, recently published a paper in which he surveyed a group of eight grandmasters and found that the group hit their highest ranking after fourteen thousand hours of practice. Even among prodigies who reached grandmaster level before the age of sixteen, we see the same pattern. Almost all of that group reached grandmaster level at fourteen or fifteen, and most started playing when they were four or five. The famous Polgár sisters (two of whom reached grandmaster status) put in somewhere north of fifty thousand hours of practice to reach the top. Epstein, similarly, argues that studies show that it takes only four thousand hours to reach “international levels” in basketball. The study in question was of a sample of players from the Australian men’s basketball team. I have nothing against either Australia or Australian basketball. But I’d be a bit more impressed if someone could find a starting point guard in the N.B.A. with fewer than ten years of basketball under his belt. Arguments about what it takes to be an elite performer are less persuasive if the performers being studied aren’t actually elite.
I think that it is also a mistake to assume that the ten-thousand-hour idea applies to every domain. For instance, Epstein uses as his main counterexample the high jumper Donald Thomas, who reached world-class level after no more than a few months of the most rudimentary practice. He then quotes academic papers making similar observations about other sports—like one that showed that people could make the Australian winter Olympic team in skeleton after no more than a few hundred practice runs. Skeleton, in case you are curious, is a sport in which a person pushes a sled as fast as she can along a track, jumps on, and then steers the sled down a hill. Some of the other domains that Epstein says do not fit the ten-thousand-hour model are darts, wrestling, and sprinting. “We’ve tested over ten thousand boys,” Epstein quotes one South African researcher as saying, “and I’ve never seen a boy who was slow become fast.”
As it happens, I have been a runner and a serious track-and-field fan my entire life, and I have never seen a boy who was slow become fast either. For that matter, I’ve never met someone who thinks a boy who was slow can become fast. Epstein has written a wonderful book. But I wonder if, in his zeal to stake out a provocative claim on this one matter, he has built himself a straw man. The point of Simon and Chase’s paper years ago was that cognitively complex activities take many years to master because they require that a very long list of situations and possibilities and scenarios be experienced and processed. There’s a reason the Beatles didn’t give us “The White Album” when they were teen-agers. And if the surgeon who wants to fuse your spinal cord did some newfangled online accelerated residency, you should probably tell him no. It does not invalidate the ten-thousand-hour principle, however, to point out that in instances where there are not a long list of situations and scenarios and possibilities to master—like jumping really high, running as fast as you can in a straight line, or directing a sharp object at a large, round piece of cork—expertise can be attained a whole lot more quickly. What Simon and Chase wrote forty years ago remains true today. In cognitively demanding fields, there are no naturals.
Photograph by Kent Skibstad/AFP/Getty.
Source: http://www.newyorker.com/online/blogs/sportingscene/2013/08/psychology-ten-thousand-hour-rule-complexity.html
Gladwell on chess and the ‘10,000 hours rule’
Malcolm Gladwell, the author of the best-selling bookOutliers, has written a piece on the website of The New Yorker dealing with some of the criticism he’s received over his popularization of the so-called ’10,000 hours’ rule that he describes in above-mentioned book.
This rule, which postulates that “the key to success in any field is, to a large extent, a matter of practicing a specific task for a total of around 10,000 hours” (Wikipedia), has been criticized for good and wrong reasons. (Gladwell himself notes in the first paragraph of his article that the number of 10,000 hours was actually suggested 40 years longer ago, by Herbert Simon and William Chase.)
As he notes in his New Yorker piece, many people have, incorrectly, interpreted the rule to mean that “with enough practice, anyone could achieve a level of proficiency that would rival that of a professional.” This is indeed not at all what Gladwell argued in his book – “achievement is talent plus preparation” he literally wrote in Outliers, in which chess plays a prominent role. Gladwell:
“In cognitively demanding fields, there are no naturals. Nobody walks into an operating room, straight out of a surgical rotation, and does world-class neurosurgery. And second—and more crucially for the theme of Outliers—the amount of practice necessary for exceptional performance is so extensive that people who end up on top need help. They invariably have access to lucky breaks or privileges or conditions that make all those years of practice possible.”
Gladwell is correct that his position is often wrongly interpreted, even in high-brow publications. A recent, oft-quoted article also seems to imply that the 10,000 hours of practice only, would be sufficient to reach a certain expert level.
Gladwell’s article can be read online here and is well worth reading in full. Unfortunately, when it comes to chess (as it inevitably does in discussions such as these!), Gladwell himself could be a lot clearer. One of the chess studies he mentions in the article is, as he says, about “chess masters” – a rather vague term in itself, as Gladwell notes as well. But then he goes on to say:
“Epstein [author of The Sports Gene – ed.] is talking about chess masters—the lowest of the four categories of recognized chess experts. (It’s Division II chess.) Grandmasters—the highest level—are a different story.”
This just doesn’t make sense. How are ‘chess masters’ in the lowest of four categories of recognized chess experts? Four? Which four? If we ignore the possibility that Gladwell and others refer to local categories, such as the American ‘master’ (a USCF rating of 2200+) or the Russian ‘Candidate Master’ (a bit more elastic), I can at best think of three ‘officially recognized’ ones: Fide Master (FM), International Master (IM), and International Grandmaster (GM).
Of these, FM is definitely not a fully ‘recognized’ title – you can buy it if at one point in your chess career you achieved a FIDE rating of 2300. Which is great, no doubt - but anyone flashing his FM title suggesting he’s some kind of real ‘chess master’ is in the real chess world gently dismissed: “see you when you have three norms and 2400, my friend!” This leaves only two ‘real’ recognized categories: IM and GM.
But there are more problems with what Gladwell writes. Whilst grandmasters are indeed officially the ‘highest level’, there are, of course, huge differences among grandmasters. This fact is ignored in Gladwell’s article (and also, it should be noted, in many other articles). In fact, these differences are, at least rating-wise, often bigger than the difference between IM and GM in the first place.
This is why it’s misleading to just state that grandmasters are ‘a different story’. An average GM is probably more similar to an average IM than he is to the likes of Vladimir Kramnik or Magnus Carlsen (whose picture accompanies the New Yorker article) – who are nevertheless also grandmasters. You just can’t lump the two together because they fit into the same official title category – especially not when you are criticizing someone else who’s doing the same. It’s simply a whole different ball game, and you need to make that distinction absolutely clear.
Similarly, he notes that “[t]he famous Polgár sisters (two of whom reached grandmaster status) put in somewhere north of fifty thousand hours of practice to reach the top.” This may be so, but there’s still a huge difference in level between Judit and Susan (about 200 points, which is again more that the difference between an average IM and an average GM). This important nuance is left out in Gladwell’s argument (even though it actually supports his broader theory that there’s more to chess expertise than just the number of hours put into it!)
Gladwell goes on to talk about running – a subject he claims to know a lot about as he’s done it himself. This might well be true, and what he writes certainly makes sense from an intuitive point of view, but it’s hard not to be skeptical when what he writes about chess is incomplete at best.
Source: http://www.chessvibes.com/gladwell-on-chess-and-the-‘10000-hours-rule’;